Join us   Log in   editor@jmedicalcasereports.org  


FRONTIERS IN MEDICAL CASE REPORTS - Volume 1; Issue 3, (May-Jun, 2020)

Pages: 1-17
Print Article   Download XML  Download PDF

Correlation between Impulsiveness, Cortical Thickness and Slant of The Forehead in Healthy Adults

Author: J. David Guerrero Apolo

Category: Health Care

Abstract:

Impulsiveness is a multidimensional psychological construct with high clinical interest because it is a risk factor for several psychiatric disorders. This study was designed to investigate structural correlates of self-reported impulsiveness, measured by using the Short Scale of Impulsive Behaviour, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale and Zuckerman Sensation Seeking Scale V. As a secondary objective, and based on preliminary findings concerning the positive relationship between impulsiveness and the slant of the forehead degrees (SFD), we explore this relationship and the correlation between SFD and cortical thickness (CT) of the entire cortex. From a sample of 48 participants' structural magnetic resonance images, three self-reports measuring impulsiveness and SFD were obtained. CT of the whole cortex was obtained for each participant through Freesurfer. Correlations between impulsiveness and CT and between CT and SFD were analyzed. Correlations between impulsiveness and SFD were also analyzed. Results showed significant adjusted and corrected correlations, both negative and positive, between impulsiveness and CT. Five negative associations with fronto-temporal and occipital areas were found. Of the 15 positive correlations, eight were with the right anterior cingulate cortex and three with left superior frontal gyrus. Positive correlations between 14 impulsiveness scores and SFD were also found. In conclusion, CT in prefrontal and temporal areas influences self-reported impulsiveness in healthy adults. Furthermore, SFD could influence the CT of regions involved in impulsiveness. Finally, we suggest that a higher SFD higher self-reported impulsiveness in healthy adults.

Keywords: Impulsiveness, Cortical thickness, Prefrontal cortex, FreeSurfer

DOI URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.47746/FMCR.2020.1310

Full Text:

Introduction

Impulsiveness can be defined as “predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internal or external stimuli without regard to the negative consequences of these reactions” (Moeller et al., 2001). Presently, it is regarded as a multidimensional psychological construct (Caswel et al., 2015) included in most explanatory models of personality (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001). Impulsiveness can be easily observed in people (Barratt, 1984) and, furthermore, it holds great clinical interest since high levels constitute a risk factor for many psychiatric disorders (Altszuler et al., 2016). It has been associated with the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Tajima-Pozo et al., 2015), with the borderline personality disorder (O’Neill et al., 2013), antisocial behaviour (Maneiro et al., 2017), substance abuse (Gullo et al., 2014; Marín-Mayor et al., 2014), bipolar disorder (Ozten et al., 2015) and addiction to online gambling (Du et al., 2016). Therefore, a key question which has yet to be clarified is to define which neurobiological factors are implicated in impulsiveness (Fineberg et al., 2014). The prefrontal cortex seems to be the most involved area in the inhibitory control of conduct and the one that has generated most evidence up to now (Knutson et al., 2015).

In healthy people, negative associations by using Voxel Based Morphometry (VBM) have been found among impulsiveness and gray matter volume (GMV) of the bilateral orbitofrontal cortex (Matsuo et al., 2009; Kumari et al., 2009), the left anterior cingulate cortex (Matsuo et al., 2009) and the left orbitofrontal cortex (Schilling et al., 2013b). In contrast, some positive associations have also been found in frontal and posterior cingulate regions (Cho et al., 2013; Gardini et al., 2009). A measurement which has generated growing scientific interest in the last decade consists of evaluating the cortical thickness (CT) by surface-based morphometry (SBM), rather than with VBM (Choi et al., 2008). Research through SBM has also shown some variations in the CT in prefrontal regions in more impulsive subjects. In a systematic review of CT in people diagnosed with a bipolar disorder, a significant decrease prefrontal areas were observed (Hanford et al., 2016). Likewise, in adult patients diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, lower CT was found in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex (Makris et al., 2006). Previous studies also found that healthy subjects with higher scores in self-informed impulsiveness had lower CT in the superior frontal cortex, the medial frontal cortex and the orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex (Kumari et al., 2009; Schilling et al., 2012; Schilling et al., 2013a). More recently, it has been found, by using SBM, that higher impulsiveness traits were associated with a thinner cortex in the left superior, middle and inferior frontal cortex (Tu et al., 2017).

In short, the above-mentioned data suggest that morphological variations in prefrontal regions play a fundamental role in impulsiveness, both in healthy and clinical subjects.However, impulsiveness seems not to be solely under the control of the prefrontal cortex (Braquehais et al., 2010), since temporal regions are also implicated (Hanford et al., 2016; Lyoo et al., 2006).

Thus, our main objective is to identify structural correlates of impulsiveness in healthy subjects by exploring the CT of the entire cortex through SBM (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu). As a secondary objective and based on preliminary findings concerning the positive association between impulsiveness and the slant of the forehead (Guerrero et al., 2016; 2018a; 2018b) we explored the relationship between the slant of the forehead degree (SFD) and the CT of the entire cortex, an aspect that is yet to be studied.

Material and Methods

Participants

Two advertisements were placed in training centers and libraries of Barcelona city (Spain) in order to recruit our sample. All the subjects were right- handed. The volunteers were screened by a short private clinic interview using Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., 1998). Those with hyperactivity and attention deficit-related symptoms or a psychiatric background in first-degree relatives were excluded. In total, nine volunteers were excluded. The final sample consisted of 48 volunteers. (66.7% male) with an age mean of 36.2 years-old (SD = 9.9). Their academic level was elementary in 7 subjects (14.6%), intermediate in 16 subjects (33.3%) and university degree in 25 subjects (52.1%). Participants completed three self-reports and after that they went on to take a profile picture, whose method will be further detailed later. Finally, an appointment was made for magnetic resonance imaging. Each participant signed an informed consent before entering the study and agreed on the use of data for research purposes.

Impulsiveness measurements

Multiple scales of self-reported impulsivity were used to ensure a more representative contruct, three self-reports have been used which add up to 15 different scores.

The short scale of impulsive behaviour (UPPS-P) (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001). The Spanish version of the UPPS-P (Cándido et al., 2012) also measures five dimensions of impulsiveness: negative and positive urgency, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance and sensation seeking. The UPPS-P contains 20 items scored by a four-point Likert-type scale, where 1 means fully agree, 2 partially agree, 3 partially disagree and 4 totally disagree; higher scores reflect more impulsiveness.

The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) (Patton et al., 1995). The Spanish version of the BIS-11 (Oquendo et al., 2011) also measures impulsiveness as a personality trait. It has three sub-scales: attentional impulsiveness, motor impulsiveness, and non-planning impulsiveness. It has 30 items scored in a Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (rarely or never), 1 (occasionally), 3 (often) to 4 (always or almost always). Higher scores reflect more impulsiveness.

The Zuckerman Sensation Seeking Scale V or SSS-V (Zuckerman et al.1978) has been adapted to Spanish by Pérez and Torrubia (1986). It has 40 items and provides total scores as well as four 10-item factors each: thrill and adventure seeking (TAS), experience seeking (ES), disinhibition (DIS) and boredom susceptibility (BS). The choice was compulsory for one of the two alternatives, true or false. True answers scored one point.

Measurement of the slant forehead degrees

The profile photographs were taken by a digital reflex Canon camera model EOS 1100 D EF-S 18-55. The participants remained seated on a chair previously fixed to the floor by the researcher and all volunteers were trained to adopt a natural head position (NHP). NHP is defined as an innate, physiological and reproducible position achieved when a person is in a relaxed position, sitting or standing, looking at the horizon or at an external reference point (mirror, a point on the wall, etc.) at the same eye level (Moorrees, 1994). Therefore, the edge of the photograph was regarded as the true vertical (TV) and as a reference in the measurement of the SFD. The digital photographs were printed in DIN-A4 format. The degrees of the angle of the forehead inclination were measured by a semicircular protractor (brand Staedtler 568) with a 10cm ruler. Two anthropometric points of reference were taken from the methodology created by Farkas (1994): triquion (TR) and glabella (G). The vertex of the angle was fixed on the glabella, from which two lines were drawn. Line 1 was vertically drawn, parallel to the edge of the photograph TV and was set as 0º. Line 2 was drawn from the G to the TR. The SFD was measured as the angle, in degrees, formed by the line that goes from the G to the TR (Figure 1a). Each participant was independently measured by three experts in craniofacial morphology. The agreement reached between them was high with intraclass correlation coefficient CCI = 0.99. Average of the three SFD measurements was used in the analyses. The same method has been used in previous investigations (Guerrero et al., 2016; Guerrero-Apolo et al., 2018a: Guerrero-Apolo et al., 2018b).

Magnetic resonance imaging acquisition

Magnetic resonance imaging scans were obtained using 1.5 T (GE BRIVO). High-resolution 3D-FSPGR images, powered in T1, were taken for each participant. The acquisition parameters were the following: TE= minimum; TI= 300 ms; Flip Angle= 20º; 130 adjacent axial sections; mould 256 x 256, 25 cm FoV; Slice Thickness = 1.2 mm; Receiver Bandwidth 15.63 Khz.

Structural MRI data preprocessing: Data was analyzed using Freesurfer 5.3.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu). Freesurfer provides automated algorithms for the volumetric segmentation of subcortical structures and estimation of CT (Fischl and Dale, 2000). CT is calculated as the closest distance from the gray/white boundary to the gray/cerebrospinal fluid boundary at each vertex on the tessellated surface (Fischl and Dale, 2000). All the specifications about FreeSurfer parcellations, including reliability, validity and anatomic limits, are described in Desikan, et al. (2006). The images were visually inspected so as to detect structural artifacts and abnormalities and none of the segmentations were corrected.

Statistical analysis

To analyze the association between impulsiveness measures and CT and previous verification of linearity, partial Pearson correlation coefficient was calculated. The adjustment terms considered were academic level and age. The ICC was calculated for interobserver agreement. Data was analyzed with Stata 14. Type I error was set at the usual 0.05 level. With the aim of not hiding possible relevant associations, no correction of the Type I error was applied to results presented in the tables. Nevertheless, the false discovery rate (FDR) (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) was applied to know what statistics would hold some significance after correction.

Results

Table 1 shows a description of the impulsiveness measures and the SFD. Impulsiveness measures obtained with UPPS-P and Zuckerman showed the most extreme scores, with observed minimum and maximum values matching or close to the theoretically possible. Scores of BIS-11 were less extreme.

Correlations between impulsiveness measures and cortical thickness

Table 2 shows statistically significant negative adjusted correlations between impulsiveness and CT and Table 3 shows the positives ones. Figure 1b presents the significant cerebral regions. There were six negative (three in left and three in right hemisphere) and fifteen positive (six in left and nine in right hemisphere) results. Four out of the six negatives corresponded to SSS-V boredom susceptibility. From the 15 positives eight were with the caudal anterior cingulate cortex (all in the right hemisphere) and three were with superior frontal cortex (all in the left hemisphere). After FDR correction 19 of 21 significant correlations held significant.

Correlations between slant of the forehead degree and cortical thickness

Figure 2a graphically and Figure 2b numerically present adjusted correlations between CT and SFD. Six positive associations were found statistically significant and half of them continued to be significant after FDR correction. The six correlations were very similar with values slightly above 0.30.

Correlations between slant of the forehead degree and impulsiveness

Table 4 presents statistically significant adjusted correlations between the impulsiveness measures obtained with the three questionnaires and SFD. Statistical significance was obtained for fourteen out of the fifteen correlations calculated. All significant correlations were positive, ranging from values r = .336 (UPPS-P Lack of perseverance) to r = .607 (UPPS-P Sensation seeking). After FDR correction none of the fourteen correlations lost statistical significance.

Table 1. Description of age, SFD and impulsiveness measures

N=48

Mean

SD

Minimum

Maximum

Age

36.19

9.9

23

57

Slant of the forehead degrees (SFD)

17.73

5.49

10

34

UPPS-P Total (20-80)

41.83

10.38

24

69

UPPS-P Negative urgency (4-16)

9.79

2.81

5

16

UPPS-P Positive urgency (4-16)

9.39

2.78

4

16

UPPS-P Sensation seeking (4-16)

9

2.94

4

16

UPPS-P Lack of premeditation (4-16)

7.22

2.41

4

15

UPPS-P Lack of perseverance (4-16)

6.41

2.43

4

16

BIS11 Total (0-120)

44.35

15.62

21

91

BIS11 Attention impulsiveness (0-32)

13.77

4.55

6

27

BIS11 Motor impulsiveness (0-40)

14.75

6.5

3

32

BIS11 Non-planning impulsiveness (0-48)

15.83

7.04

1

34

SSS-V Total (0-40)

20.22

7.05

5

32

SSS-V Thrill and adventure seeking (0-10)

5.45

3.08

0

10

SSS-V Excitation seeking (0-10)

6.35

2.12

2

10

SSS-V Disinhibition (0-10)

4.25

2.2

0

9

SSS-V Boredom susceptibility (0-10)

4.16

2.01

1

8

Table 2. Statistically significant negative correlations between cortical thickness and impulsiveness measures

Rostral anterior cingulate

Isthmus cingulate

Pericalcarine

Inferior temporal

Temporal pole

Right

UPPS-P

-0.310

Lack of premeditation

0.036

Left

SSS-V

-0.369

Excitation seeking

0.012*

Right

Left

Left

Right

SSS-V

-0.321

-0.338

-0.400

-0.411

Boredom susceptibility

0.030*

0.022*

0.006*

0.005*

In each cell: hemisphere, Pearson correlation (adjusted by academic level and age), uncorrected p value

*Significant after FDR correction.

Table 3. Statistically significant positive correlations between cortical thickness and impulsiveness measures

Caudal anterior cingulate

Caudal middle frontal

Precentral

Superior frontal

Transverse temporal

Right

0.041

UPPS-P Total

0.004*

Right

UPPS-P

0.308

Positive urgency

0.037

Right

Left

UPPS-P

0.499

0.371

Sensation seeking

<0.0005*

0.011*

Right

Left

UPPS-P

0.380

0.292

Lack of premeditation

0.009*

0.049*

Right

UPPS-P

0.319

Lack of perseverance

0.031*

Right

Left

BIS11 Total

0.327

0.345

0.026*

0.019*

BIS11 Attention impulsiveness

Left

Right

Left

0.361

0.302

0.402

0.017*

0.041*

0.006*

BIS11 Motor impulsiveness

Left

0.298

0.044*

BIS11 Non-planning impulsiveness

Right

0.357

0.015*

Right

SSS-V Total

0.300

0.043*

In each cell: hemisphere, Pearson correlation (adjusted by academic level and age), uncorrected p value

*Significant after FDR correction.

Table 4. Statistically significant positive correlations between impulsiveness measures and slant of the forehead degrees

Slant of the forehead degrees

UPPS-P Total

0.534

<.0005*

UPPS-P Positive urgency

0.407

.005*

UPPS-P Sensation seeking

0.607

<.0005*

UPPS-P Lack of premeditation

0.522

<.0005*

UPPS-P Lack of perseverance

0.336

.023*

BIS11 Total

0.528

<.0005*

BIS11 Attention impulsiveness

0.556

<.0005*

BIS11 Motor impulsiveness

0.453

.002*

BIS11 Non-planning impulsiveness

0.392

.007*

SSS-V Total

0.585

<.0005*

SSS-V Thrill and adventure seeking

0.395

.007*

SSS-V Excitation seeking

0.425

.003*

SSS-V Disinhibition

0.502

<.0005*

SSS-V Boredom susceptibility

0.408

.005*

In each cell: Pearson correlation (adjusted by academic level and age), uncorrected p value. *Significant after FDR correction.

Discussion

In this exploratory study we have found some significant correlations between the self-reported impulsiveness and cortical thickness in the prefrontal and temporal regions in healthy subjects. These findings are consistent with growing evidence of the influence of frontotemporal variations on impulsiveness. Furthermore, it is the first study which finds a positive correlation between the slant of forehead degree and the cortical thickness in prefrontal regions.

We also sought to obtain a higher self-reported representativeness of impulsiveness, which accounts for the 15 analyzed scores. In the exploration of the CT and its association with the UPPS-P model, we have found significant correlations in four scores. With respect to the negative ones, only the factor lack of premeditation has correlated with the right pericalcarine cortex. There exists scant evidence of the effect of pericalcarine cortex on impulsiveness, although some negative correlations have been found between the CT and the local gyrification index (LGI) in sensation seeking and attention impulsiveness (BIS) respectively (Holmes et al., 2016; Hirjak, et al., 2016). Despite the fact that the UPPS-P structural correlations are hardly known, this finding backs up former studies on significant morphological variations in the right pericalcarine cortex in terms of the presence of impulsive traits. However, we must point out that lack of premeditation has not been able to stand a statistical significance correction (FDR, p < 0.05).

In the UPPS-P questionnaire the positive correlations found must be highlighted. In particular the total scores and factors such as positive urgency, sensation seeking, lack of premeditation and lack of perseverance have correlated with the CT in the right caudal anterior cingulate cortex and, except positive urgency, all the above-mentioned factors have stood an FDR correction. These findings are in agreement with previous studies which, by using VBM, also found positive correlations between impulsiveness factors and GMV in right superior and middle frontal cortex, medial prefrontal cortex and posterior and anterior cingulate regions (Gardini et al., 2009; Cho et al., 2013). In the standard FreeSurfer atlas (Desikan et al., 2006), the caudal anterior cingulate cortex is part of the anterior cingulate cortex, whose region establishes significant associations between impulsive traits and morphological variations (Gardini et al., 2009; Matsuo et al., 2009; Thomann et al., 2015). Furthermore, the cingulate cortex has been associated with control of impulses, novelty seeking, persistence and impulsive behaviour (Bechara, 2005; Gardini et al., 2009). This finding may suggest that an increase in the CT of the right caudal anterior cingulate cortex exerts an influence on impulsiveness assessed with the UPPS-P model and, consequently, one might think of a consistent neuro-anatomical correlate found for this model.

On the other hand, the sensation seeking factor has also positively correlated with the left caudal middle frontal cortex. This region seems to be involved with cognitive control (Goghari and MacDonald, 2008) and inhibition of impulsive decisions (Essex et al., 2012). From this perspective it’s surprising that the left caudal middle frontal cortex has solely correlated with the sensation seeking factor out of the 15 scores analyzed. This seems to suggest a modest influence of the CT of the middle frontal cortex on the impulsive traits depicted in this study. Contrary to this finding, however, a negative association (Holmes et al., 2016) has previously been observed, which leads us to be very cautious about drawing conclusions. Furthermore, although the UPPS-P model is made up of our very different traits (Sharma et al. 2013) our exploration shows consistent evidence of a common neuroanatomical structure (right caudal anterior cingulate cortex) in five out of six scores of the model analyzed. Unlike some previous studies (Boes et al., 2009; Matsuo et al., 2009; Schilling et al., 2012; Tu et al., 2017) We did not find any negative associations between the CT and BIS. Our results show positive correlations (after FDR correction for all the scores) in influencing areas in the inhibitory control of impulsive conduct (superior frontal cortex and caudal anterior cingulate cortex). Which is consistent with earlier studies where the same association was found in prefrontal (Gardini et al., 2009; Cho et al., 2013; Du et al., 2016) and temporal (Schilling et al., 2012) areas. However, the heterogeneity of results of previous studies makes it difficult to draw some conclusions about the relationship between the variability in CT of prefrontal areas and BIS. Despite that our results in BIS seem to back up the evidence, independent of the CT, of the critical role of prefrontal and temporal structures. The correlation between the BIS total score with right caudal anterior cingulate cortex and left superior frontal cortex contrasts with the negative association found in left superior frontal cortex (Tu et al., 2017), which suggests, along with our findings, that variations in the left superior frontal cortex influence impulsiveness assessed with BIS total score. Another score, attention impulsiveness, not only correlates with the left superior frontal cortex, but also with the bilateral precentral cortex, a region which seems to be implicated in inhibitory control (Ma et al., 2012). This observation concerning the precentral cortex is consistent with previous studies, where positive associations with the impulsiveness in an adolescent sample were found by using VBM (Schilling et al., 2013b). The same association was found in the LGI in precentral and postcentral regions (Hirjak et al., 2016). Another factor that has correlated with the left superior frontal cortex has been motor impulsiveness. This finding is contrary to previous observations (Matsuo et al., 2009; Schilling et al., 2012). Even though our sample is comparable to previous studies, the factorial structure of the BIS-11 has been troublesome and the results derived from this self-report must be cautiously interpreted (Reid et al., 2013). Our findings in BIS show that variations in CT, in the left superior frontal cortex, can become an influencing structural correlation. Furthermore, the only scores that has not correlated with superior frontal cortex has been non-planning impulsiveness, an observation that is consistent with a previous study (Schilling et al. 2012). However, this factor has indeed correlated with the right caudal anterior cingulate cortex, where positive correlations have been found by using VBM (Cho et al., 2013; Gardini et al., 2009) as well as some variations in the LGI of frontal areas (Hirjak et al., 2016). This seems to indicate that regions of the cingulate cortex also exert their influence on non-planning impulsiveness.

Our latest self-report analyzed (SSS-V) showed four negative and one positive correlations, all of them in different brain regions (corrected results), which confirmed the higher number of negative correlations found in the three self-reports analyzed. With respect to the negative correlations, we have found a correlation between the ES and the CT in the left rostral anterior cingulate cortex. This finding can be expected given the influence of the cingulate cortex on impulsiveness traits (Gardini et al., 2009; Matsuo et al., 2009). What we found in the BS factor is more unexpected because of its correlation with four brain regions (right isthmus cingulate, right temporal pole, left temporal pole and left inferior temporal). This factor showed no correlations with the other three factors that make up SSS-V (Zuckerman et al., 1978). Together with our finding, this suggests that BS may be an independent factor where reductions in the CT of temporal and posterior regions of the cingulate cortex could be implicated in this trait. Therefore findings in right temporal pole, left temporal pole and left inferior temporal cortex support former studies on the importance of temporal regions in the manifestation of impulsive traits (Gardini et al., 2009; Hanford et al., 2016; Hirjak et al., 2016; Schilling et al., 2012; Schilling et al., 2013b), including general impulsiveness (Lyoo et al., 2006). They also support the evidence that impulsiveness is not exclusively under the control of the prefrontal cortex (Braquehais et al., 2010).

On the other hand, TAS is regarded as a non-impulsive way of seeking sensations (Zuckerman, 1983). Oddly enough, TAS did not correlate with any brain structures in our study. This finding can support Zuckerman’s TAS and back up the idea of the uses of the factors ES and BS as an equivalent measurement for impulsiveness. Although the sensation seeking trait and impulsiveness have distinct developmental trajectories (Steinberg et al., 2008), they can share a common neuroanatomical structure as well as underlie the same neurobiology (Holmes et al., 2016). Considering the influence exerted by impulsiveness on the sensation seeking trait (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1985), it would back up the use of SSS-V as an equivalent measurement to impulsiveness.

On the other hand, SSS-V total score has only correlated with right caudal anterior cingulate cortex (corrected result), which supports the evidence shown in this study concerning the influence of this region on the impulsive traits. In this sense, it is worth highlighting that right caudal anterior cingulate cortex has correlated with eight scores, including the total score of the three self-reports analyzed.

With respect to our secondary objective, this is the first study that explores the SFD with the CT of the entire cortex. Our finding of the positive correlations between SFD and the CT in prefrontal regions (Figure 2) with self-reported impulsivity supports the growing body of scientific evidence of the influence of these regions on the self-reported impulsiveness. Based on our data it seems that SFD can influence the CT in frontal but not posterior regions of the brain. However, the complexity in discussing these findings is obvious and more studies need to be carried out in order to draw consistent conclusions. It appears that the frontal lobes influence the shape of the forehead and a higher development in of the frontal protuberance (bulky superciliaries) results in a higher slant (Balueva and Lebedinskaya, 1991). Likewise, subjects with protruding superciliar structure seem to report structure seem to report a higher obstinacy in their behaviour (Han and Park, 2014). This observation is consistent with Pujol, et al. (2011), where it was found that more compulsive subjects, therefore, possibly more impulsive (Voltas-Moreso et al., 2013) presented with a lengthening pattern in the space of the cerebral-spinal fluid of the frontal operculum which correlated positively with the frontal protuberance (superciliary arches). Together with our finding, it is necessary to point out that previous studies suggest that the structure of the face can facilitate information about the brain (Sisodiya et al., 2008) or that the face, the craniofacial skeleton and the central nervous system have an interconnected development (Kjaer, 1995).

Some limitations to our study must be duly considered. First, we did not control the consumption of tobacco or other substances A factor that is strongly associated with externalization or disinhibitory disorders (alcohol dependence, drug taking or behaviour disorders) is negative urgency of the model UPPS-P (Settles et al., 2012), where no correlation was found.

Second, the size of the sample was modest, which limits the generalization of our results. However, the number of participants is similar to comparable former previous studies (Kumari et al., 2009; Schilling et al., 2012).

Third, our study has focused on subjective impulsiveness measurements and, despite being described as an efficient method (Mathias et al., 2008), it would be convenient to replicate or refute our findings with objective measurements since there exists the evidence of no correlation between objective measurements and GMV in frontal areas (Tschernegg et al., 2015). That is why we must be cautious and limit our findings to the instruments used rather than to impulsiveness broadly speaking.

Finally, despite the existence of individual differences in the profile view of the slant of the forehead (Adams et al., 2013; Oh et al., 2016; Guerrero et al., 2016; Guerrero et al., 2018a,) an aspect that can trigger some controversy is the anthropometric measurement SFD. However, a recent study found a high level of interobserver agreement in the visual assessment of the level of the forehead slant, whose measurement obtained a high correlation with the anthropometric SFD measurement (Guerrero et al., 2016; Guerrero et al., 2018a). This backs up the validity of the anthropometric SFD measurement used in this study. Furthermore, in our SFD measurement the NHP has been taken into account, usually used in profile photographs in order to control the placement of the reference points in teleradiography (Lundstrom, et al., 1995).

In conclusion, our exploration supports the growing evidence of the importance of the CT variations in frontal and temporal areas in the assessment of self-reported impulsiveness among healthy subjects. If we consider that the morphology of craniofacial structures and that of the brain is interwoven in a complex fashion (DeMyer, 1975), then we can suggest a relationship between the slant of the forehead and the CT variations in frontal regions. However, this preliminary finding must be taken cautiously and we must and observe the results of future investigations. We suggest that a new line of research could be opened about the importance of SFD in the manifestation of the impulsive behavior in healthy subjects, as well as about the relationship between SFD and structural and functional aspects of the underlying brain tissue. Thus, this study examined the cortical thickness correlates of auto-report impulsiveness, demonstrating that prefrontal and temporal regions are crucial for impulsiveness in healthy adults. Likewise, the slant of the forehead is associated with the thickness of crucial cortical regions for impulsiveness traits.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Abbreviations: BIS-11: Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; BS: Boredom Susceptibility; CT: Cortical Thickness; DIS: Disinhibition; ES: Experience Seeking; G: Glabella; GMV: Gray Matter Volume; NHP: Natural Head Position; TAS: Thrill And Adventure Seeking; TR: Triquion; SBM: Surface-Based Morphometry; UPPS-P: Short Scale of Impulsive Behaviour; SFD: Slant Of The Forehead Degree; SSS-V: Zuckerman Sensation Seeking Scale V; TV: True Vertical; VBM: Voxel Based Morphometry

Acknowledgements

We are really grateful to Javier González Zabaleta, CEO in Medimsight, for his professionalism. Also, our heartfelt thanks to the developer LAIMBIO, from the URJC. To be grateful for the contribution of Dr. J B. Navarro-Pastor in the analysis of data. And also, to be thankful for the support of Dr. Julián Gabarre Mir in part of the sample recruitment, and magnetics resonances acquisition. As well as, to all the volunteers who participated in this study.

References:

Altszuler AR, Page TF, Gnagy EM, Coxe S, Arrieta A, Molina BS, Pelham WE. Financial Dependence of Young Adults with Childhood ADHD. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2016; 44: 1217-1229

Balueva TS and Lebedinskaya GV. Anthropological reconstruction. Academy of Sciences. Moscow. Russian, 1991.

Barratt ES. Impulsiveness and aggression. In Violence and Mental Disorder: Developments in Risk Assessments (ed J. Monahan and H.J. Steadman), pp. 61–79. I. L. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1994.

Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of will power to resist drugs a neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci 2005; 8: 1458–1463.

Benjamini Y and Hochberg Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J Royal STAT SOCB (Methodological) 1995; 7: 289-300.

Bjork JM, Momenan R, Hommer DW. Delay Discounting Correlates with Proportional Lateral Frontal Cortex Volumes. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 65: 710-713.

Boes AD, Bechara A, Tranel D, Anderson SW, Richman L, Nopoulos P. Right ventromedial prefrontal cortex: a neuroanatomical correlate of impulse control in boys, SCAN 2009; 4: 1–9.

Braquehais MD, Ramos-Quiroga JA, Sher L. Impulsivity: Current and future trends in pharmacological treatment. Expert Rev Neurother 2010; 10: 1367–1369

Cándido A, Orduña E, Perales JC, Verdejo-García A, Billieux J. Validation of a short Spanish version of the UPPSP impulsive behaviour scale. Trastornos Adictivos 2012; 14: 73–78.

Caswell AJ, Bond R, Duka T, Morgan MJ. Further evidence of the heterogeneous nature of impulsivity. Pers Indiv Differ 2015; 76: 68–74.

Cho SS, Pellecchia G, Aminian K, Ray N, Segura B, Obeso I, Strafella AP. Morphometric correlation of impulsivity in medial prefrontal cortex. Brain Topogr 2013; 26: 479–87.

Choi YY, Shamosh NA, Cho SH, DeYoung CG, Lee MJ, Lee JM, Kim SI, Cho ZH, Kim K, Gray JR, Lee KH. Multiple bases of human intelligence revealed by cortical thickness and neural activation. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 10323–10329.

DeMyer W. Median facial malformations and their implications for brain malformations. Birth Defects Orig Artic Ser 1975; 11: 155-181.

Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, Albert MS. An auto- mated labeling system for subdi- viding the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 2006; 3: 968–980.

Du X, Qi X, Yang Y, Du G, Gao P, Zhang Y, Qin W, Li X, Zhang Q. Altered Structural Correlates of Impulsivity in Adolescents with Internet Gaming Disorder. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10: 1–7.

Eysenck HJ and Eysenck MW. Personality and individual differences: a natural science approach. New York: Plenum Press. 1985.

Essex BG, Clinton SA, Wonderley LR, Zald DH. The impact of the posterior parietal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices on the optimization of long-term versus immediate value. J Neurosci 2012; 32: 15403–15413.

Farkas LG. Anthropometry of the Head and Face. Raven Press: New York, 1994.

Fineberg NA, Chamberlain SR, Goudriaan AE, Stein DJ, Vanderschuren LJ, Gillan CM, Shekar S, Gorwood PA, Voon V, Morein-Zamir S, Denys D. New developments in human neurocognition: clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity. CNS Spectrums 2014; 19: 69-89.

Fischl B and Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 2000; 97: 11050–11055.

Goghari VM and MacDonald AW. Effects of varying the experimental design of a cognitive control paradigm on behavioral and functional imaging outcome measures. J Cognitive Neurosci 2008; 20: 20–35.?

Fineberg NA, Chamberlain SR, Goudriaan AE, Stein DJ, Vanderschuren LJ, Gillan CM, Shekar S, Gorwood PA, Voon V, Morein-Zamir S, Denys D. Impulsivity and its Association with the Slant of the Forehead. Int J of Morphol 2016; 34: 990–996.

Guerrero-Apolo JD, Navarro-Pastor JB, Bulbena-Vilarrasa A, Gabarre-Mir J. The slant of the forehead as a craniofacial feature of impulsiveness. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria 2018a; 40: 270-276.

Guerrero-Apolo JD, Navarro-Pastor JB, Bulbena-Vilarrasa A, Gabarre-Mir J. Association between self-reported impulsiveness and gray matter volume in healthy adults. An exploratory MRI study. Neuroscience Letters 2018b; 674: 112-116.

Gullo MJ, Loxton NJ, Dawe S. Impulsivity: Four ways five factors are not basic to addiction. Addict Behav 2014; 39: 1547–1556.

Han DS and Park JH. Aesthetic Correction of a Protrusive Forehead through Repositioning of the Anterior Wall of the Frontal Sinus. Arch Craniofac Surg 2014; 15: 129–132.

Hanford LC, Nazarov A, Hall GB, Sassi RB. Cortical thickness in bipolar disorder: A systematic review. Bipolar Disord 2016; 18: 4-18.

Hirjak D, Thomann AK, Kubera KM, Wolf RC, Jeung H, Maier-Hein KH, Thomann PA. Cortical folding patterns are associated with impulsivity in healthy young adults. Brain Imaging Behav 2016; 1: 1–12.

Holmes AJ, Hollinshead MO, Roffman JL, Smoller JW, Buckner RL. Individual Differences in Cognitive Control Circuit Anatomy Link Sensation Seeking, Impulsivity, and Substance Use. J Neurosci 2016; 36: 4038-4049.

Kjaer I. Human prenatal craniofacial development related to brain development under normal and pathologic conditions. Acta Odontol Scand 1995; 53: 135–143.

Kumari V, Barkataki I, Goswami S, Flora S, Das M, Taylor P. Dysfunctional, but not functional, impulsivity is associated with a history of seriously violent behaviour and reduced orbitofrontal and hippocampal volumes in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2009; 173: 39–44.

Knutson KM, Dal Monte O, Schintu S, Wassermann EM, Raymont V, Grafman J, Krueger F. Areas of brain damage underlying increased reports of behavioral disinhibition. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2015; 27: 193–198.

Lyoo IK, Pollack MH, Silveri MM, Ahn KH, Diaz CI, Hwang J, Kim SJ, Yurgelun-Todd DA, Kaufman MJ, Renshaw PF. Prefrontal and temporal gray matter density decreases in opiate dependence. Psychopharmacology 2006; 184: 139–144.

Lundström A, Lundström F, Lebret LM, Moorrees CF. Natural head position and natural head orientation: basic considerations in cephalometric analysis and research. Eur J Orthodont 1995; 18: 111–120.

Ma Y, Hu H, Agmon A. Short-term plasticity of unitary inhibitory-to-inhibitory synapses depends on the presynaptic interneuron subtype. J Neurosci 2012; 32: 983-988.

Makris N, Biederman J, Valera EM, Bush G, Kaiser J, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ. Cortical thinning of the attention and executive function networks in adults with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Cerebral Cortex 2006; 17: 1364-1375.

Maneiro L, Gómez-Fraguela JA, Cutrín O, Romero E. Impulsivity traits as correlates of antisocial behaviour in adolescents. Pers Indiv Differ 2017 104: 417–422.

Marín Mayor M, Jurado-Barba R, Martínez-Grass I, Ponce Alfaro G, Rubio Valladolid G. La respuesta de sobresalto y la inhibición prepulso en los trastornos por uso de alcohol: implicaciones para la práctica clínica. Clínica y Salud 2014; 25: 147-155.

Mathias CW, Marsh?Richard DM, Dougherty DM. Behavioral measures of impulsivity and the law. Behav Sci Law 2008; 26: 691–707.

Matsuo K, Nicoletti M, Nemoto K, Hatch JP, Peluso MA, Nery FG, Soares JC. A voxel-based morphometry study of frontal gray matter correlates of impulsivity. Hum Brain Mapp 2009; 30: 1188–1195.

Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiat 2001; 158: 1783–1793.

Moorrees CF. Natural head position – a revival. AJO-DO 1994; 105: 512–513.

Oh MJ, Kang SJ, Jeon SP, Sun H. Forehead Inclination in the Lateral Profile in Koreans. Arch Plast Surg 2016; 43: 293–295.

O’Neill A, D’Souza A, Carballedo A, Joseph S, Kerskens C, Frodl T. Magnetic resonance imaging in patients with borderline personality disorder: a study of volumetric abnormalities. Psychiat Res 2013; 213: 1-10.

Oquendo MA, Baca-Garcia E, Graver R, Morales M, Montalvan V. Spanish adaptation of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11). Eur J Psychiat 2001; 15: 147–155.

Ozten M, Erol A, Karayilan S, Kapudan H, Orsel ES, Kumsar NA. Impulsivity in bipolar and substance use disorders. Compr Psychiat 2015; 59: 28–32.

Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J Clin Psychol 1995; 51: 768–774.

Pe?rez J and Torrubia R. Fiabilidad y validez de la versio?n espan?ola de la escala de bu?squeda de sensaciones, forma V. Rev Amt Lat Psicol 1986; 18: 7–22.

Pujol J, Soriano?Mas C, Gispert JD, Bossa M, Reig S, Ortiz H, Alonso P, Cardoner N, López?Solà M, Harrison BJ, Deus J. Variations in the shape of the frontobasal brain region in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Hum Brain Mapp 2011; 32: 1100-1108.

Schilling C, Kühn S, Romanowski A, Schubert F, Kathmann N, Gallinat J. Cortical thickness correlates with impulsiveness in healthy adults. NeuroImage 2012; 59: 824–830.

Schilling C, Kühn S, Paus T, Romanowski A, Banaschewski T, Barbot A, Barker GJ, Brühl R, Büchel C, Conrod PJ, Dalley JW. Cortical thickness of superior frontal cortex predicts impulsiveness and perceptual reasoning in adolescence. Mol Psychiatry 2013a; 18: 624–630.

Schilling C, Kühn S, Romanowski A, Banaschewski T, Barbot A, Barker GJ, Brühl R, Büchel C, Charlet K, Conrod PJ, Czech K. Common structural correlates of trait impulsiveness and perceptual reasoning in adolescence. Hum Brain Mapp 2013b; 34: 374–383.

Settles RE, Fischer S, Cyders MA, Combs JL, Gunn RL, Smith GT. Negative urgency: A personality predictor of externalizing behavior characterized by neuroticism, low conscientiousness, and disagreeableness. J Abnorm Psychol 2012; 121: 160–172.

Sharma L, Markon KE, Clark LA. Toward a theory of distinct types of “impulsive” behaviors: a meta-analysis of self-report and behavioral measures. Psychol Bull 2013; 140: 374–408.

Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiat 1998; 59: 22–33; quiz 34–57.

Sisodiya S. Brain structure, function, and genetics revealed by studies of the eye and face. Curr Opin Neurol 2008; 21: 404–409.

Steinberg L, Albert D, Cauffman E, Banich M, Graham S, Woolard J. Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Dev Psychol 2008; 44: 1764–778.

Tajima-Pozo K, Ruiz-Manrique G, Yus M, Arrazola J, Montañes-Rada F. Correlation between amygdala volume and impulsivity in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Neuropsychiatry 2015; 27: 362–367.

Thomann PA, Hirjak D, Kubera KM, Stieltjes B, Wolf RC. Neural network activity and neurological soft signs in healthy adults. Behav Brain Res 2015; 278: 514–519.

Tschernegg M, Pletzer B, Schwartenbeck P, Ludersdorfer P, Hoffmann U, Kronbichler M. Impulsivity relates to striatal gray matter volumes in humans: evidence from a delay discounting paradigm. Front Hum Neurosci 2015; 9: 384.

Tu PC, Kuan YH, Li CT, Su TP. Structural correlates of trait impulsivity in patients with bipolar disorder and healthy controls: a surface-based morphometry study. Psychol Med 2017; 47: 1292–1299.

Moreso NV, Hernández-Martínez C, Val VA, Sans JC. Socio-demographic and psychopathological risk factors in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Epidemiologic study of school population. Int J Clin Hlth Psyc 2013; 43: 118-126.

Whiteside SP and Lynam DR. The five factormodel and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers Indiv Differ 2001; 30: 669-89.

Zuckerman M, Eysenck SB, Eysenck HJ. Sensation seeking in England and America: Crosscultural, age, and sex comparisons. J Consult Clin Psychol 1978; 46: 139–149.

Zukerman M. A biological theory of sensation seeking. In Biological bases of sensation seeking, impulsivity, and anxiety. (ed. M. Zukerman), pp. 37-76. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum, 1983.